Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorHirnstein, Marco
dc.contributor.authorAndrews, Lisa Coloma
dc.contributor.authorHausmann, Markus
dc.date.accessioned2014-12-08T13:26:49Zen_US
dc.date.accessioned2014-12-09T08:36:14Zen_US
dc.date.accessioned2014-12-10T09:59:45Z
dc.date.available2014-12-10T09:59:45Z
dc.date.issued2014-06-13eng
dc.identifier.issn0004-0002
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/1956/8878
dc.description.abstractSex differences in specific cognitive abilities are well documented, but the biological, psychological, and sociocultural interactions that may underlie these differences are largely unknown. We examined within a biopsychosocial approach how gender stereotypes affect cognitive sex differences when adult participants were tested in mixed- or same-sex groups. A total of 136 participants (70 women) were allocated to either mixed- or same-sex groups and completed a battery of sex-sensitive cognitive tests (i.e., mental rotation, verbal fluency, perceptual speed) after gender stereotypes or gender-neutral stereotypes (control) were activated. To study the potential role of testosterone as a mediator for group sex composition and stereotype boost/threat effects, saliva samples were taken before the stereotype manipulation and after cognitive testing. The results showed the typical male and female advantages in mental rotation and verbal fluency, respectively. In general, men and women who were tested in mixed-sex groups and whose gender stereotypes had not been activated performed best. Moreover, a stereotype threat effect emerged in verbal fluency with reduced performance in gender stereotyped men but not women. Testosterone levels did not mediate the effects of group sex composition and stereotype threat nor did we find any relationship between testosterone and cognitive performance in men and women. Taken together, the findings suggest that an interaction of gender stereotyping and group sex composition affects the performance of men and women in sex-sensitive cognitive tasks. Mixed-sex settings can, in fact, increase cognitive performance as long as gender-stereotyping is prevented.en_US
dc.language.isoengeng
dc.publisherSpringereng
dc.rightsAttribution CC BYeng
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0eng
dc.subjectGender stereotypeseng
dc.subjectMental rotationeng
dc.subjectVerbal fluencyeng
dc.subjectGroup sex compositioneng
dc.subjectStereotype threateng
dc.titleGender-stereotyping and cognitive sex differences in mixed- and same-sex groupseng
dc.typePeer reviewed
dc.typeJournal article
dc.date.updated2014-12-08T13:26:49Zen_US
dc.description.versionpublishedVersion
dc.rights.holderCopyright 2014 The Authors
dc.identifier.doihttps://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-014-0311-5
dc.identifier.cristin1154050
dc.source.journalArchives of Sexual Behavior
dc.source.4043
dc.source.148
dc.source.pagenumber1663-1673
dc.subject.nsiVDP::Social sciences: 200::Psychology: 260::Biological psychology: 261eng
dc.subject.nsiVDP::Social sciences: 200::Psychology: 260::Cognitive psychology: 267eng
dc.subject.nsiVDP::Samfunnsvitenskap: 200::Psykologi: 260::Biologisk psykologi: 261nob
dc.subject.nsiVDP::Samfunnsvitenskap: 200::Psykologi: 260::Kognitiv psykologi: 267nob


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

Attribution CC BY
Except where otherwise noted, this item's license is described as Attribution CC BY